Aug 24 2005
A new U.S. study has found that it is possible that by just thinking that a medicine will relieve pain is enough to prompt the brain to release its own natural painkillers, and soothe painful sensations.
The study by the University of Michigan has come up with the first direct evidence that the brain’s own pain-fighting chemicals, endorphins, play a role in the phenomenon known as the placebo effect, and this response corresponds with a reduction in feelings of pain.
It has been shown in previous studies that the brain reacts physically when a person is given a sham pain treatment, which they believe will help them.
This new study, by a team from the U-M Molecular and Behavioral Neurosciences Institute, however, is the first to pinpoint a specific brain chemistry mechanism for a pain-related placebo effect, and may help explain why so many people say they get relief from therapies and remedies with no actual physical benefit.
It hopefully will also lead to better use of cognitive, or psychological, therapy for people with chronic pain.
Lead author Jon-Kar Zubieta, M.D., Ph.D., associate professor of psychiatry and radiology at the U-M Medical School and associate research scientist at MBNI, says they were able to see that the endorphin system was activated in pain-related areas of the brain, and that activity increased when someone was told they were receiving a medicine to ease their pain; they then reported feeling less pain.
Zubieta says the mind-body connection is quite clear, and the results are a serious blow to the idea that the placebo effect is a purely psychological, not physical, phenomenon.
The findings are based on sophisticated brain scans from 14 young healthy men who agreed to allow researchers to inject their jaw muscles with a concentrated salt water solution to cause pain.
The injection was made while they were having their brains scanned by a positron emission tomography (PET) scanner.
During one scan, they were told they would receive a medicine (in fact, a placebo) that might relieve pain.
Then every 15 seconds during the scans, they were asked to rate the intensity of their pain sensations on a scale of 0 to 100, and they gave more detailed first-person ratings after the experiment.
The researchers correlated the participants’ ratings with their PET scan images, which were made using a technique that reveals the activity of the brain’s natural painkilling endorphin chemicals, also called endogenous opioids which stop the transmission of pain signals from one nerve cell to the next.
Because the endorphin system naturally tries to quell pain whenever it occurs, the researchers slowly increased the amount of concentrated salt water being injected in the muscle as the scans continued, in order to keep the participants’ rating of their pain within the same point range throughout the experiment.
The placebo, a small amount of hydrating solution, was then given intravenously every four minutes.
As the researchers alerted participants that the placebo was coming, and injected the placebo dose, the amount of additional concentrated salt water needed to maintain participants’ pain over time increased - indicating a reduction in pain sensitivity that the subjects were not aware of.
So, the thought that they were getting a pain drug, actually allowed the participants to tolerate even more pain-inducing concentrated salt water than before.
After each scan, the participants were asked more questions about their mood, emotions and other aspects of how they felt during the scans. Significant differences were seen between post-scan ratings given by participants after the scan in which they received the placebo, and after the scan during which they received the jaw injection alone.
Nine of the participants were classified as "high placebo responders" because they had more than a 20 percent difference between pain and placebo scans in their average pain ratings per volume of salt water infused, in other words, the placebo effect was strong.
The other five were classified as "low placebo responders".
Because the new study was done only in healthy men between the ages of 20 and 30, further research will be needed to determine whether the effect occurs in women and in people with various illnesses. The power of placebos to ease pain symptoms has been well-documented in many groups of subjects and illnesses, but the researchers started with healthy young males to rule out the impact of chronic pain, mood disorders and hormone variations, which can also affect the endorphin system.
The research was funded by the National Institutes of Health.
The results will be published in the August 24 issue of the Journal of Neuroscience.