Counting helps infants relate things to numbers

Infants and animals are able to understand approximate quantities of different types, in terms of relative proportions of the numbers of different sets of objects, provided the difference in numbers is large. This is represented by approximate number systems (ANS) in a rough way.

On the other hand, humans who have learned numbering can represent numbers by means of exact representations in the form of integers. Now a new study published in the journal Developmental Science shows that tiny tots less than 2 years old can actually relate counted quantities to approximate numbers. These toddlers expect a one-to-one correspondence with the counted total, when the counted objects are first hidden and then some removed.

Children do not understand the meanings of count words like “two” and “three” until the preschool years. But even before knowing the meanings of these individual words, might they recognize that counting is “about” the dimension of number?  Image Credit: Ksana Kuzmina / Shutterstock
Children do not understand the meanings of count words like “two” and “three” until the preschool years. But even before knowing the meanings of these individual words, might they recognize that counting is “about” the dimension of number? Image Credit: Ksana Kuzmina / Shutterstock

Integers are capable of showing small differences between the numbers of objects in different sets, but require that the individual learn to count numbers by name. Children were thought to begin this process, called linguistic counting, by around 2.5 years – reciting numbers up to 3. Over the next six months they learn the meaning of the number ‘one’, the same period for the number ‘two’, and then ‘three’ over another six months. Thus by about age 4 they realize that the numbers mean an exact count and that the last number counted is the cardinality of (or number of items in) that set.

However, there is a link between inexact and exact representations of numbers, as shown by the verbal estimates of the number of things in an unspecified array. In doing so, the individual is using an exact representation to describe an inexact number. In Western cultures, the earliest links between these two systems begins in the second year, with children being aware when counting is not done correctly, as by skipping around or by not following the right order. However, they are able to connect and use individual number terms to describe uncounted sets of objects only by age 4.

In the present paper, the researchers explored the idea that until this age infants have a partial idea that a number word means something to do with the count or quantity of a set of objects, though not the exact meaning. They used a search task to find out if infants 14-20 month old would be able to assess if objects hidden in a box had all been removed or any left behind, by assessing their search activity. Devising 5 separate experiments, they found that counting does help infants to represent an approximate array of objects numerically.

Older findings

Prior research showed that infants 12-22 months looked for the remaining objects if 2 or less out of an array of up to 3 objects were removed in their sight. That is, they recognized that one or more was still hidden, depending on the quantity removed. On the other hand, with more than 3 objects, they were unable to assess the quantity removed; even if only 2 out of 4 objects were removed, they did not search the box for the remaining objects. In other words, they were using precise representations of individual objects, which is why they cannot go beyond 3, the limit of their working memory, in preference to imprecise approximations of number representations which can go to any upper limit.

The study

The present study aimed to answer if counting objects before hiding them in the box would help infants switch to approximate number representations that can go beyond working memory limits. The researchers used a black box about 1.1 feet long, with a covered slit through which the babies could reach inside without seeing whether or how many objects were hidden in the box.

Experiment 1

There were 4 of two different kinds of objects. In contrast to the earlier research, when 4 similar objects were counted before being hidden in the box, when the experimenter removed 2, the babies remembered more hidden objects and continued to search longer for the rest. They did not appear to remember exactly how many objects were counted, but used an approximate idea. This was not seen when 4 objects were hidden without counting first. This means that infants used the counting done by the experimenter to assess whether the ‘right’ quantity of objects had been removed.

Experiment 2

To confirm the role of the ANS rather than of individual object counting in this increased ability to understand the difference in arrays of 4 objects when they were counted before being hidden, the experiment was carried out with either 2 or 3 of the 4 hidden objects being retrieved before measuring the period of searching. Babies searched longer when 2 were removed but not after 3, indicating they cannot differentiate between 3 and 4 by ANS – both are represented as equivalent in the babies’ minds. This indicates that ANS rather than integer counting is at work.

Experiment 3

Experiment 3 tried to confirm this by using a larger number in each array, namely, 6 objects, which is well beyond the capacity of the working memory but can be represented by the ANS. In the first case, the babies should react to 6 as to 4, and in either case they should not search longer for hidden objects after 3 are removed. In the second case, they can distinguish between 4 and 6 in terms of ratio and thus should look longer for the remaining hidden objects if counting is first performed before removing 3 objects. The second case was shown to be true in this part of the experiment. Unlike experiment 2, in the case of the objects counted before being hidden, babies searched longer when 3 out of 6 objects were removed, but not when 3 of 4 were retrieved.

Experiment 4

To confirm the role of the ANS and not working memory, the experimenters then looked at younger infants of 14 months, a group in which 6 hidden objects have never been represented successfully. Astonishingly, these young infants also searched longer after hearing 6 objects counted before being hidden in the box followed by removal of 3 or 4 of the objects. They were able to represent the approximate number of objects even without being able to count them, to a level (6) which cannot be explained by working memory.

Experiment 5

A final experiment used proper names rather than count names, to rule out the possibility that the babies might be differentiating and thus remembering the objects by means of the count names rather than by a rough numerical approximation. However, this showed that searching was longer only when 1 object remained out of 2 hidden objects, but not when 2 remained out of 4. Thus a labeling effect was not a contributor to the successful approximation of the number of objects seen after counting in the earlier experiments.

Implications

This study therefore shows the earliest link ever observed between ANS and integer counting. Researcher Jenny Wang says, “When we counted the toys for the babies before we hid them, the babies were much better at remembering how many toys there were.” Thus when an infant between 1 and 2 years observes objects being counted, this may attract the attention to the concept of numeric measurement, or the use of numbers to represent the quantity of objects. This can occur a long time before the meaning of count words themselves is understood.

The ability to notice that counting refers to the number rather than the names of objects might be due to early experience of observing counting by others. Counting may thus help in the development of number representations in infants at an earlier stage than ever thought before. And count names are not confused with proper names or labels. Thus even young infants have an advanced understanding of what the world around them is like, and what the various words people use around them actually mean. Later research may show how this is associated with later number skills.

Journal reference:

Wang, J. J., Feigenson, L. Infants recognize counting as numerically relevant. Dev Sci. 2019; 22:e12805. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12805, https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/desc.12805

Dr. Liji Thomas

Written by

Dr. Liji Thomas

Dr. Liji Thomas is an OB-GYN, who graduated from the Government Medical College, University of Calicut, Kerala, in 2001. Liji practiced as a full-time consultant in obstetrics/gynecology in a private hospital for a few years following her graduation. She has counseled hundreds of patients facing issues from pregnancy-related problems and infertility, and has been in charge of over 2,000 deliveries, striving always to achieve a normal delivery rather than operative.

Citations

Please use one of the following formats to cite this article in your essay, paper or report:

  • APA

    Thomas, Liji. (2019, October 28). Counting helps infants relate things to numbers. News-Medical. Retrieved on November 02, 2024 from https://www.news-medical.net/news/20191028/Counting-helps-infants-relate-things-to-numbers.aspx.

  • MLA

    Thomas, Liji. "Counting helps infants relate things to numbers". News-Medical. 02 November 2024. <https://www.news-medical.net/news/20191028/Counting-helps-infants-relate-things-to-numbers.aspx>.

  • Chicago

    Thomas, Liji. "Counting helps infants relate things to numbers". News-Medical. https://www.news-medical.net/news/20191028/Counting-helps-infants-relate-things-to-numbers.aspx. (accessed November 02, 2024).

  • Harvard

    Thomas, Liji. 2019. Counting helps infants relate things to numbers. News-Medical, viewed 02 November 2024, https://www.news-medical.net/news/20191028/Counting-helps-infants-relate-things-to-numbers.aspx.

Comments

The opinions expressed here are the views of the writer and do not necessarily reflect the views and opinions of News Medical.
Post a new comment
Post

While we only use edited and approved content for Azthena answers, it may on occasions provide incorrect responses. Please confirm any data provided with the related suppliers or authors. We do not provide medical advice, if you search for medical information you must always consult a medical professional before acting on any information provided.

Your questions, but not your email details will be shared with OpenAI and retained for 30 days in accordance with their privacy principles.

Please do not ask questions that use sensitive or confidential information.

Read the full Terms & Conditions.

You might also like...
Iron supplements may boost brain development in children with HIV