Nasty neighbor effect: People compete more with those in their group than outsiders

In a recent study published in the journal Science Advances, behavioral scientists in Europe examined whether people belonging to a group based on common language, nationality, or political ideology exhibited in-group favoritism in two situations of competition—one where they were trying to outcompete other in-group members and the other when they were trying to prevent being outcompeted by an in-group member.

Study: The nasty neighbor effect in humans. Image Credit: Andrii Yalanskyi / ShutterstockStudy: The nasty neighbor effect in humans. Image Credit: Andrii Yalanskyi / Shutterstock

Background

Behavioral science studies have found that individuals who belong to groups bonded by commonalities in language, country of origin, or political ideology are more cooperative with members of their group than strangers or members of other groups. This tendency, often referred to as parochialism or in-group favoritism, can be observed the world over. An inference from observations of in-group favoritism would be that members of a group would be more willing to compete with outsiders than with members of their group.

However, most evidence for in-group favoritism comes from studies that have not independently examined the effects of competition. People within a group are also more inclined to cooperate with in-group members while expending fewer resources when they can gain benefits that might come at a cost to others. Nonetheless, the absence of cooperation does not directly equate to the presence of competition, and cooperation within a group does not imply lower competition towards members of the group.

About the study

In the present study, the researchers aimed to understand whether in-group favoritism could be observed among participants from 51 countries during two scenarios of conflict—one in which the individual is the competitor (attacker) and the other in which the individual is on the defense, trying to prevent being outcompeted (defender).

For the first part of the study, the researchers obtained data from close to 13,000 participants from 51 countries, at the approximate rate of 250 individuals from each society. The participants were stratified by gender and age. An online survey was used to conduct the experiment, with the researchers designing the survey in English and providing professionally translated versions to the non-English speaking participants.

The experiment involved each participant facing randomly selected opponents from different countries and making 54 independent decisions about investing standardized monetary units (MU), with half of the decisions being made as the attacker and the other half as the defender.

The individual was informed about the opponent’s nationality just before they made the decision. For each block of 27 decisions, one involved an interaction with an opponent of the same nationality, one was with an unidentified individual, and the remaining 25 were with individuals of different nationalities.

For the second part of the study, the same experiment was conducted among 552 participants residing in Nairobi, Kenya, but belonging to different ethnocultural groups. This part of the study was to study interactions that could be generalized beyond the online interactions seen between individuals of different countries.

Kenya was chosen as the location for this part of the study because of its history of interethnic armed conflict, which is believed to have political roots. The researchers examined interactions between two communities with a history of conflict, such as the Kikuyu and Luo, as well as two communities that have been at peace with each other, such as the Luhaya and Kamba.

In-group cooperators and nasty neighbors in minimal groups. (A and C) Experimental setup of the nested social dilemma (top; stage 1, A) with an attack option (bottom; stage 2, C). (B) Between-group competition favors the emergence of in-group favoritism. Bar chart showing in-group favoritism and universal cooperation as mean percentage of the endowment contributed when competition is absent versus present. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. (D) Status differences favor the emergence of within-group nastiness. Floodlight plot showing the regions of differences in status of the target of attack (x axis, standardized) for which the effect of in-group versus out-group (y axis) on attack becomes significant. The vertical lines in the floodlight plot show the exact values at which significance begins and ends. Blue lines indicate significance at 5% level. (E) Relative differences in perceived competition favor the emergence of a nasty neighbor effect. Scatterplot shows the association between perceived competition toward in-group members (minus out-group) and the nasty neighbor effect (i.e., attack of in-group members minus out-group members).

In-group cooperators and nasty neighbors in minimal groups. (A and C) Experimental setup of the nested social dilemma (top; stage 1, A) with an attack option (bottom; stage 2, C). (B) Between-group competition favors the emergence of in-group favoritism. Bar chart showing in-group favoritism and universal cooperation as mean percentage of the endowment contributed when competition is absent versus present. **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001. (D) Status differences favor the emergence of within-group nastiness. Floodlight plot showing the regions of differences in status of the target of attack (x axis, standardized) for which the effect of in-group versus out-group (y axis) on attack becomes significant. The vertical lines in the floodlight plot show the exact values at which significance begins and ends. Blue lines indicate significance at 5% level. (E) Relative differences in perceived competition favor the emergence of a nasty neighbor effect. Scatterplot shows the association between perceived competition toward in-group members (minus out-group) and the nasty neighbor effect (i.e., attack of in-group members minus out-group members).

Major findings

The study found that while individuals of a group tend to cooperate with and trust members of their group, they also exhibit a tendency to compete, investing more in competing with in-group members than outsiders.

The researchers called this behavior the ‘nasty neighbor effect’ and found that individuals exhibited this behavior in situations involving investments in the attacker-defender contest. Additionally, a significant proportion of the participants also exhibited the ‘nasty neighbor effect’ in situations involving the prisoner’s dilemma game theory, where two individuals can cooperate for a mutual benefit, or one individual betrays the other for an individual reward.

The study found cultural variations in the ‘nasty neighbor effect’, correlating with egalitarian and hierarchical values, as well as with wealth. The researchers also discussed how the ‘nasty neighbor effect’ is not restricted to human societies and has been observed in other species, such as birds that live in groups or colonies, social insects, black-crested gibbons, Eurasian beavers, Diana monkeys, and banded mongoose, suggesting that this behavior might have evolutionary roots.

Conclusions

Overall, the findings indicate that in-group favoritism is not universally pervasive and that while cooperation with in-group members is largely beneficial, an individual might independently exhibit competitive behavior with in-group members in specific contexts. This ‘nasty neighbor’ behavior is independent of the cooperation and trust within groups and often emerges in situations of resource scarcity.

Journal reference:
Dr. Chinta Sidharthan

Written by

Dr. Chinta Sidharthan

Chinta Sidharthan is a writer based in Bangalore, India. Her academic background is in evolutionary biology and genetics, and she has extensive experience in scientific research, teaching, science writing, and herpetology. Chinta holds a Ph.D. in evolutionary biology from the Indian Institute of Science and is passionate about science education, writing, animals, wildlife, and conservation. For her doctoral research, she explored the origins and diversification of blindsnakes in India, as a part of which she did extensive fieldwork in the jungles of southern India. She has received the Canadian Governor General’s bronze medal and Bangalore University gold medal for academic excellence and published her research in high-impact journals.

Citations

Please use one of the following formats to cite this article in your essay, paper or report:

  • APA

    Sidharthan, Chinta. (2024, July 01). Nasty neighbor effect: People compete more with those in their group than outsiders. News-Medical. Retrieved on October 31, 2024 from https://www.news-medical.net/news/20240701/Nasty-neighbor-effect-People-compete-more-with-those-in-their-group-than-outsiders.aspx.

  • MLA

    Sidharthan, Chinta. "Nasty neighbor effect: People compete more with those in their group than outsiders". News-Medical. 31 October 2024. <https://www.news-medical.net/news/20240701/Nasty-neighbor-effect-People-compete-more-with-those-in-their-group-than-outsiders.aspx>.

  • Chicago

    Sidharthan, Chinta. "Nasty neighbor effect: People compete more with those in their group than outsiders". News-Medical. https://www.news-medical.net/news/20240701/Nasty-neighbor-effect-People-compete-more-with-those-in-their-group-than-outsiders.aspx. (accessed October 31, 2024).

  • Harvard

    Sidharthan, Chinta. 2024. Nasty neighbor effect: People compete more with those in their group than outsiders. News-Medical, viewed 31 October 2024, https://www.news-medical.net/news/20240701/Nasty-neighbor-effect-People-compete-more-with-those-in-their-group-than-outsiders.aspx.

Comments

The opinions expressed here are the views of the writer and do not necessarily reflect the views and opinions of News Medical.
Post a new comment
Post

While we only use edited and approved content for Azthena answers, it may on occasions provide incorrect responses. Please confirm any data provided with the related suppliers or authors. We do not provide medical advice, if you search for medical information you must always consult a medical professional before acting on any information provided.

Your questions, but not your email details will be shared with OpenAI and retained for 30 days in accordance with their privacy principles.

Please do not ask questions that use sensitive or confidential information.

Read the full Terms & Conditions.

You might also like...
Researchers develop decision tree to help clinicians predict genetic diagnoses in neurodevelopmental cases