How music lessons can shape children's social and emotional growth

In a recent study published in the journal PLOS One, researchers at the University of Southern California, Los Angeles, investigated the association between formal music instruction during childhood and the subsequent development of socioemotional, rhythmic, and pitch-matching skills. They conducted a longitudinal case-control cohort study comparing 83 children enrolled in either music-, sports-, or no-after-school instruction programs.

Results revealed that participants in music instruction programs outperformed their control-cohort peers in pitch-matching ability. These findings suggest that long-term formal music education programs can positively influence certain aspects of children's socioemotional development, though the overall impact is limited.

Study: Long-term music instruction is partially associated with the development of socioemotional skills. Image Credit: Photo joy / ShutterstockStudy: Long-term music instruction is partially associated with the development of socioemotional skills. Image Credit: Photo joy / Shutterstock

Background

Formal music instruction is the systematic education of instrumental technique and music theory. Previous research has suggested its positive benefits in improving students' pitch-matching (the ability to mimic the pitch of an external audio cue) and rhythmic entrainment (the ability to synchronize internal rhythms with externally perceived ones) aptitudes.

Kuther (2022) postulated that music instruction may promote social and emotional functioning. Studies also elucidate the social roles of pitch-matching and rhythmic entrainment in establishing social bonds, effective communication, collective identity development, and cooperation.

Separate bodies of literature have investigated the links between music education and cognition or self-esteem. Unfortunately, these studies often investigate one-on-one music tutoring and fail to explore the impacts of group-based music education programs that frequently comprise after-school activities for schoolchildren.

The present study aims to address these knowledge gaps by assessing 1. Do children exposed to after-school training programs differ in their pitch-matching ability, socioemotional aptitude, and rhythmic entrainment capacity compared to those who are not? 2. What is the association between pitch-matching and rhythmic entrainment and socioemotional outcomes?

About the study

Study data was derived from a seven-year-long cohort study of children between the ages of five and eight years (mean = 6.81) from the greater Los Angeles area, United States (US) of America. Participants comprised three cohorts – music (Youth Orchestra; n = 26), sports (soccer or swimming; n = 28), or control (no-after-school activity; n = 29) education programs.

Research instruments comprised pitch-matching tasks, rhythmic entrainment evaluations, sharing tasks, and cognitive/empathy assessments. Pitch-matching was evaluated using a singing task in which participants were required to mimic the pitch (10 melodies, each 5-9 notes long) of vocally trained experimenters. Audio recordings of experimenters and participants were compared using the methodologies of Pfordresher and Brown (2007) via pitch-matching (hertz [HZ]) comparisons, scored between 0 and 1.

Rhythmic entrainment was evaluated under 'alone' and 'social' scenarios. In the former, participants were provided a drum and were required to beat it to match a prerecorded audio rhythm (120- or 180 beats per minute [bpm]). In the latter, a trained investigator joined participants in drumming to the prerecorded beat. Accuracy and synchronization were evaluated using the methodologies of Kirschner and Tomasello (2009) and scored between 0-1.

Sharing was assessed using a variation of the dictator game, which evaluated the willingness of children to share 10-15 stickers they had received with a pictured stranger without any personal benefit. Empathy (state, trait, and theory of mind) and cognitive variables were assessed using the Index of Empathy for Children and Adolescents (IECA), the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test of theory of mind (Eyes Test), and the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI-II) subsets, respectively.

Statistical differences between cohorts were computed using Principal Component Analysis (PCA) and linear mixed effects models.

Pitch-matching task stimuli. Musical score showing the melodies sung during the task. Each numbered section represents a melody the participant first heard then echoed back to the experimenter. Sections 1–3, 4–6, 7–8, and 9–10 were grouped into four musical phrases during analysis.Pitch-matching task stimuli. Musical score showing the melodies sung during the task. Each numbered section represents a melody the participant first heard then echoed back to the experimenter. Sections 1–3, 4–6, 7–8, and 9–10 were grouped into four musical phrases during analysis. 

Study findings

Pitch-matching analysis revealed significant differences between music and other cohort participants – the former averaged 0.13 points higher than the sports group and 0.26 higher than the controls. No effect of time (years) was noted in this criterion.

While rhythmic scores did not differ between cohorts at the start of the study (year 1), all groups showed per-year improvements in scores. Improvements were more significant in the music group.

Tests of socioemotional skills revealed that the sports group was the most likely to share, followed by the music group (9% less), with the controls performing the poorest. However, all groups showed yearly improvements in these skills (~7.2% increase yearly).

Most empathy tests gave confounding results at baseline, but the results of the Eyes test improved over the years. Once again, improvements were most significant in the music group.

Conclusions

Contrasting previous research, the present study did not find improvements in pitch-matching over time, most likely due to the complexity of the singing activity used herein compared to previous studies. In contrast, empathy and, most substantially, rhythmic entrainment improved over the years.

In most tests (except sharing and certain empathy measures), music cohort participants were observed to outperform their peers. Together, these findings highlight the benefits of formal music training in children's socioemotional development, with the strength of benefits dependent on the similarity of the domain to music.

Journal reference:
Hugo Francisco de Souza

Written by

Hugo Francisco de Souza

Hugo Francisco de Souza is a scientific writer based in Bangalore, Karnataka, India. His academic passions lie in biogeography, evolutionary biology, and herpetology. He is currently pursuing his Ph.D. from the Centre for Ecological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science, where he studies the origins, dispersal, and speciation of wetland-associated snakes. Hugo has received, amongst others, the DST-INSPIRE fellowship for his doctoral research and the Gold Medal from Pondicherry University for academic excellence during his Masters. His research has been published in high-impact peer-reviewed journals, including PLOS Neglected Tropical Diseases and Systematic Biology. When not working or writing, Hugo can be found consuming copious amounts of anime and manga, composing and making music with his bass guitar, shredding trails on his MTB, playing video games (he prefers the term ‘gaming’), or tinkering with all things tech.

Citations

Please use one of the following formats to cite this article in your essay, paper or report:

  • APA

    Francisco de Souza, Hugo. (2024, July 21). How music lessons can shape children's social and emotional growth. News-Medical. Retrieved on October 31, 2024 from https://www.news-medical.net/news/20240721/How-music-lessons-can-shape-childrens-social-and-emotional-growth.aspx.

  • MLA

    Francisco de Souza, Hugo. "How music lessons can shape children's social and emotional growth". News-Medical. 31 October 2024. <https://www.news-medical.net/news/20240721/How-music-lessons-can-shape-childrens-social-and-emotional-growth.aspx>.

  • Chicago

    Francisco de Souza, Hugo. "How music lessons can shape children's social and emotional growth". News-Medical. https://www.news-medical.net/news/20240721/How-music-lessons-can-shape-childrens-social-and-emotional-growth.aspx. (accessed October 31, 2024).

  • Harvard

    Francisco de Souza, Hugo. 2024. How music lessons can shape children's social and emotional growth. News-Medical, viewed 31 October 2024, https://www.news-medical.net/news/20240721/How-music-lessons-can-shape-childrens-social-and-emotional-growth.aspx.

Comments

The opinions expressed here are the views of the writer and do not necessarily reflect the views and opinions of News Medical.
Post a new comment
Post

While we only use edited and approved content for Azthena answers, it may on occasions provide incorrect responses. Please confirm any data provided with the related suppliers or authors. We do not provide medical advice, if you search for medical information you must always consult a medical professional before acting on any information provided.

Your questions, but not your email details will be shared with OpenAI and retained for 30 days in accordance with their privacy principles.

Please do not ask questions that use sensitive or confidential information.

Read the full Terms & Conditions.

You might also like...
Abnormal BMI in children linked to impaired lung function