Study shows how human brains navigate physical space and keep track of others' location

For the first time, scientists have recorded how our brains navigate physical space and keep track of others' location. Researchers used a special backpack to wirelessly monitor the brain waves of epilepsy patients as each one walked around an empty room hunting for a hidden, two-foot spot.

In an article published in Nature, the scientists report that the waves flowed in a distinct pattern suggesting that each individual's brain had mapped out the walls and other boundaries.

Interestingly, each participant's brain waves flowed in a similar manner when they sat in the corner of the room and watched someone else walk around, suggesting these waves were also used to track other people's movements. The study was part of the NIH's Brain Research through Advancing Innovative Neurotechnologies® (BRAIN) Initiative.

We were able to directly study for the first time how a person's brain navigates an actual physical space that is shared with others. Our results suggest that our brains may use a common code for knowing where we and others are in social settings."

Nanthia Suthana, Ph.D, Study Senior Author and Assistant Professor of Neurosurgery and Psychiatry at David Geffen School of Medicine ,University of California, Los Angeles

Dr. Suthana's lab studies how the brain controls learning and memory. In this study, her team worked with a group of participants with drug-resistant epilepsy, 31-52 years old, whose brains have been surgically implanted with electrodes to control their seizures.

The electrodes reside in a memory center in the brain called the medial temporal lobe, which is also thought to control navigation, at least in rodents.

Over the past half century, scientists, including three Nobel Prize winners, discovered - in experiment after experiment - that neurons in this lobe, known as grid cells and place cells, act like a global positioning system. Moreover, the scientists found that low frequency waves of neural activity by these cells, called theta rhythms, help rodents know where they and others are while running through a maze or swimming around a shallow pool of water.

"Several pieces of indirect evidence support the medial temporal lobe's role in how we navigate. But testing these ideas any further has been technically difficult," said Matthias Stangl, Ph.D., a postdoctoral scholar at UCLA and the lead author of the article.

This study provides the most direct evidence to date supporting these ideas in humans and it was made possible by a special backpack which Dr. Suthana's team invented as part of an NIH BRAIN Initiative project.

"Many of the most important breakthroughs in brain research have been sparked by technological advances. This is what the NIH BRAIN Initiative is all about. It challenges researchers to create new tools and then use those tools to revolutionize our understanding of the brain and brain disorders," said John Ngai, Ph.D., director of the NIH's BRAIN Initiative.

At its core, the backpack contained a computer system which can wirelessly connect to the electrodes surgically implanted in a patient's head. Recently, the researchers showed that the computer can simultaneously be hooked up to several other devices including virtual reality goggles, eye trackers, and heart, skin, and breathing monitors.

"Until now the only ways to directly study human brain activity required a subject to be still, either lying in a massive brain scanner or hooked up to an electrical recording device.

In 2015, Dr. Suthana came to me with an idea for solving this problem and so we took a chance on making a backpack," said Uros Topalovic, M.S., a graduate student at UCLA and an author of the study. "The backpack frees the patient and allows us to study how the brain works during natural movements."

To examine the role the medial temporal lobe plays in navigation the researchers asked the research participants to put on the backpack and enter an empty 330-square-foot room.

Each wall was lined with a row of five colored signs numbered 1 through 5, one color per wall. Through a ceiling mounted speaker, a computerized voice asked the patient to walk to one of the signs. Once they arrived at the sign the voice then asked them to search for a two-foot in diameter spot hidden somewhere in the room. Meanwhile the backpack recorded the patient's brain waves, paths through the room, and eye movements.

Initially, each person needed several minutes to find the spot. During subsequent trials, the time shortened as their memory of the spot's location improved.

The electrical recordings revealed a distinct pattern in brain activity. The theta rhythms flowed stronger - with higher peaks and lower valleys - when the participants approached a wall than when they wandered in the middle of room.

This happened exclusively when they were searching for the spot. In contrast, the researchers saw no correlation between theta rhythm strength and location when the participants followed directions for walking to the colored signs on the wall.

"These results support the idea that under certain mental states theta rhythms may help the brain know where boundaries are located. In this case it's when we are focused and searching for something," said Dr. Stangl.

Further analysis supported this conclusion and helped rule out the possibility that the results were caused by other factors, such as activity associated with different eye, body, or head movements.

Curiously, they saw similar results when the participants watched someone else search for a spot. In these experiments, the participants would sit on a chair in the corner of the room with their backpacks on and their hands resting near a keyboard. The patients knew the location of the hidden spot and they pressed a button on the keyboard whenever the other person arrived at it.

Again, the participant's brain waves flowed strongest when the other person either neared a wall or the spot and this pattern only appeared when the person was on the hunt rather than following specific directions.

"Our results support the idea that our brains may use these wave patterns to put ourselves in another person's shoes," said Dr. Suthana. "The results open the door to helping us understand how our brains control navigation and, possibly, other social interactions."

Dr. Suthana's team plans to explore these ideas in greater depth. In addition, the team has made the backpack available to other researchers who want to learn more about the brain and brain disorders.

This year more than 175 research groups have received funding from the NIH in support of a wide variety of projects ranging from mapping the neural circuits that control what an octopus sees to helping people paralyzed by spinal cord injuries regain movements by upgrading computer programs that drive neural stimulation devices.

Source:
Journal reference:

Stangl, M., et al. (2020) Boundary-anchored neural mechanisms of location-encoding for self and others. Nature. doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-03073-y.

Comments

The opinions expressed here are the views of the writer and do not necessarily reflect the views and opinions of News Medical.
Post a new comment
Post

While we only use edited and approved content for Azthena answers, it may on occasions provide incorrect responses. Please confirm any data provided with the related suppliers or authors. We do not provide medical advice, if you search for medical information you must always consult a medical professional before acting on any information provided.

Your questions, but not your email details will be shared with OpenAI and retained for 30 days in accordance with their privacy principles.

Please do not ask questions that use sensitive or confidential information.

Read the full Terms & Conditions.

You might also like...
Specific redox protein identified as a critical regulator of ferroptosis