How can framing COVID-19 vaccine side-effects affect an individual's willingness to be vaccinated?

Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) spread rapidly across the globe, leading to over 4.9 million deaths and throwing many countries into economic crises as they attempted to control the spread of the disease with lockdowns, social distancing, and other restrictions.

Study: Effects of Side-Effect Risk Framing Strategies on COVID-19 Vaccine Intentions in the United States and the United Kingdom: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Image Credit: Yuriy2012/ ShutterstockStudy: Effects of Side-Effect Risk Framing Strategies on COVID-19 Vaccine Intentions in the United States and the United Kingdom: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Image Credit: Yuriy2012/ Shutterstock

This news article was a review of a preliminary scientific report that had not undergone peer-review at the time of publication. Since its initial publication, the scientific report has now been peer reviewed and accepted for publication in a Scientific Journal. Links to the preliminary and peer-reviewed reports are available in the Sources section at the bottom of this article. View Sources

Mass vaccination schemes have begun to help bring the disease under control, but with variants of concern on the rise, worries for the elderly and immunocompromised have returned. The stagnating vaccination rates further exacerbate these worries in the United States and the United Kingdom. Experts believe that these may be due to rising cases of vaccine hesitancy, driven by misinformation, private online groups, and certain public figures' actions.

Researchers from the University of Munich have investigated how framing vaccine side-effects can affect an individual's willingness to be vaccinated. A preprint version of the group's study is available on the medRxiv* server while the article undergoes peer review.

Background

Traditionally, vaccines are made from inactivated/attenuated forms of the virus that allow the immune system to recognize viral surface proteins with little threat. However, suppose the inactivated virus reactivates, or an attenuated strain picks up new traits through horizontal gene transfer. In that case, the virus can again become a threat. mRNA vaccines avoid this issue - they use the host's cell machinery to transcribe an mRNA strand encoding a viral surface protein.

In the case of severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2), this is usually the receptor-binding domain (RBD) of the spike protein S1 subunit. This is the usual target because the spike protein is responsible for a large portion of SARS-CoV-2 pathogenicity. The RBD binds to angiotensin-converting enzyme 2 (ACE2) to allow viral cell entry, and the N-terminal domain of the S2 subunit is responsible for cell fusion.

Vaccine hesitancy can rise from varied causes. Recent studies have identified perceived low risk of COVID-19 infection, concerns over how quickly the vaccine was developed, and antagonistic responses to perceived over-reaching government authority. One common finding showed vaccine hesitancy was driven in many individuals by the media's widespread coverage of vaccine side effects during April and May 2021, as the first vaccines were being distributed.

The study

The researchers identified how these side effects were presented could have large consequences on their perception. For example, showing infographics, using descriptive labels such as 'very low/high risk' using percentages or frequencies, and even comparing risks to more commonly understood examples, such as the risk of vaccine side effects against the risk of car crashes.

The researchers gathered nearly 9000 people to investigate their theories. One of the most simple and yet effective strategies was adding a descriptive label next to the numerical risk - a 'very low risk' label increased an individual's intention of vaccination by 3.0 percentage points.

Two different methods were used for comparing risks of side effects to more commonly understood risks - comparing to car crash mortality and comparing the risk of side effects to the risk of COVID-19 mortality. Both showed significant effects on vaccine hesitancy, but interestingly the car crash comparison was more effective. This could be due to the more commonly understood risk or the belief that the COVID-19 pandemic is less serious than in reality.

The researchers' third strategy, framing these comparisons in relative rather than absolute terms, showed no effect whatsoever on an individual's willingness to be vaccinated. In addition to this, the researchers found that the two effects that worked could also work synergistically, showing a much higher likelihood of reducing vaccine hesitancy when applied simultaneously.

Conclusion

The authors highlight the value of their study in discovering effects that could help reduce COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy at a key time in the pandemic. A large proportion of the population is vaccinated, but worrying variants such as the Delta strain are becoming increasingly able to evade this immunity. Suppose the disease is able to incubate in unvaccinated individuals long enough for a variant to emerge that can fully evade vaccine-induced immunity. In that case, it could lead to the re-establishment of many restrictions. The researchers have identified several easily implementable, scalable, and cheap strategies that can help lower the rates of vaccine hesitancy and meaningfully affect vaccine hesitancy at a population level.

This news article was a review of a preliminary scientific report that had not undergone peer-review at the time of publication. Since its initial publication, the scientific report has now been peer reviewed and accepted for publication in a Scientific Journal. Links to the preliminary and peer-reviewed reports are available in the Sources section at the bottom of this article. View Sources

Journal references:

Article Revisions

  • Apr 29 2023 - The preprint preliminary research paper that this article was based upon was accepted for publication in a peer-reviewed Scientific Journal. This article was edited accordingly to include a link to the final peer-reviewed paper, now shown in the sources section.
Sam Hancock

Written by

Sam Hancock

Sam completed his MSci in Genetics at the University of Nottingham in 2019, fuelled initially by an interest in genetic ageing. As part of his degree, he also investigated the role of rnh genes in originless replication in archaea.

Citations

Please use one of the following formats to cite this article in your essay, paper or report:

  • APA

    Hancock, Sam. (2023, April 29). How can framing COVID-19 vaccine side-effects affect an individual's willingness to be vaccinated?. News-Medical. Retrieved on November 15, 2024 from https://www.news-medical.net/news/20211018/How-can-framing-COVID-19-vaccine-side-effects-affect-an-individuals-willingness-to-be-vaccinated.aspx.

  • MLA

    Hancock, Sam. "How can framing COVID-19 vaccine side-effects affect an individual's willingness to be vaccinated?". News-Medical. 15 November 2024. <https://www.news-medical.net/news/20211018/How-can-framing-COVID-19-vaccine-side-effects-affect-an-individuals-willingness-to-be-vaccinated.aspx>.

  • Chicago

    Hancock, Sam. "How can framing COVID-19 vaccine side-effects affect an individual's willingness to be vaccinated?". News-Medical. https://www.news-medical.net/news/20211018/How-can-framing-COVID-19-vaccine-side-effects-affect-an-individuals-willingness-to-be-vaccinated.aspx. (accessed November 15, 2024).

  • Harvard

    Hancock, Sam. 2023. How can framing COVID-19 vaccine side-effects affect an individual's willingness to be vaccinated?. News-Medical, viewed 15 November 2024, https://www.news-medical.net/news/20211018/How-can-framing-COVID-19-vaccine-side-effects-affect-an-individuals-willingness-to-be-vaccinated.aspx.

Comments

The opinions expressed here are the views of the writer and do not necessarily reflect the views and opinions of News Medical.

While we only use edited and approved content for Azthena answers, it may on occasions provide incorrect responses. Please confirm any data provided with the related suppliers or authors. We do not provide medical advice, if you search for medical information you must always consult a medical professional before acting on any information provided.

Your questions, but not your email details will be shared with OpenAI and retained for 30 days in accordance with their privacy principles.

Please do not ask questions that use sensitive or confidential information.

Read the full Terms & Conditions.

You might also like...
Cleveland Clinic presents new findings on triple-negative breast cancer vaccine