Current jargon on antibiotic resistance fails to stick or scare, study finds

In a recent study published in Communications Medicine, researchers evaluate the effectiveness of current antimicrobial resistance (AMR)-related terms in public health communication based on their memorability, risk association, and linguistic attributes.

Study: Existing terminology related to antimicrobial resistance fails to evoke risk perceptions and be remembered. Image Credit: pics five / Shutterstock.com

Background 

AMR is a growing healthcare crisis, as it caused nearly five million deaths in 2019, which surpassed the fatalities from the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) in 2020. AMR occurs when certain microorganisms like bacteria and viruses evolve to resist treatments like antibiotics.

The overuse of antibiotics, influenced by factors like risk aversion in healthcare and growth motives in agriculture, aggravates AMR. Despite the severity of AMR, public awareness remains low, with media coverage often overshadowing AMR with more relatable health threats or events like the COVID-19 pandemic.

Although there is a need for more effective AMR communication, terms like "antimicrobial resistance" are perceived as abstract and challenging, while in media, "antibiotic resistance" and "superbugs" are most commonly used, and "AMR" is least used. Scholars, however, propose consistently using terms like "drug-resistant infections" for clarity.

Further research is needed, as current AMR-related terminology has shown inconsistency and inefficacy in public health communication. This has led to insufficient awareness and understanding among the general public despite the significant public health effects of AMR.

About the study 

The University of Leicester School of Psychology and Vision Sciences Ethics Committee conducted two studies, ensuring ethical compliance and informed consent from all participants. In Study 1, conducted in May 2020, 305 United States participants were sourced from Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk).

Although MTurk has historically been a rich source of diverse, quality data, recent issues arose with inattentive participants and "bots." Despite the growth in its participant pool during the initial phase of the COVID-19 pandemic, concerns over data quality remained.

To address this, the researchers enforced rigorous data screening and initiated a subsequent survey through Prolific, a platform known for superior data quality. In November 2021, Study 2 enlisted 998 United Kingdom participants from Prolific who resembled the national demographic profile.

Both studies explored word effectiveness and considered factors such as risk association and memorability across 40 health terms, including six terms related to AMR. Predictive factors for word effectiveness included linguistic measures like familiarity, processing fluency, and pronounceability.

Other controls, like demographic details and medical history, were also examined. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) was used to assess cognitive abilities, as prior research indicated its relevance in word memory.

All participants engaged with an online questionnaire. In Study 1, health terms were presented in varied sequences, while in Study 2, the order was randomized for each participant.

The list included globally recognized health threats, with terms like "COVID-19," "acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS)," and "superbugs." Participants rated two non-health-related terms, inserted as attention checks, based on several criteria and completed a vocabulary test from the WAIS.

A final memory test presented the original terms alongside 40 new ones, in which participants were asked to recall which terms they had seen before. On average, the survey took 10-20 minutes, with a one-hour time limit for Study 2.

Data were meticulously scrutinized for authenticity and consistency. Statistical analyses involved various techniques tailored to the nature of the data, thereby ensuring reliability and validity in the results.

Study findings 

In two studies comparing the risk association ratings and memory scores for 40 health risk and disease terms, six terms related to AMR consistently ranked low in both risk perception and memorability. More specifically, high-risk perception scores were attributed to terms like "cancer," "Ebola," and "heart disease," with "cancer" achieving a notable mean score of 6.65 out of seven in the second study.

Comparatively, terms such as "chicken pox," "AMR," and "diarrhea" received the lowest risk ratings. In particular, three AMR-related terms, including "antimicrobial resistance," "bacterial resistance," and "AMR," ranked among the least associated with risk.

Statistical analyses revealed significant disparities in risk association ratings for AMR terms in both studies. Interestingly, "drug-resistant infections" received higher risk association scores as compared to other AMR-related terms.

Regarding memorability, conditions like "diarrhea," "human immunodeficiency virus (HIV)," and "AIDS" were most remembered, with an impressive 96% of participants in the second study recalling them correctly. In stark contrast, AMR-related terms such as "AMR," "antimicrobial resistance," and "drug-resistant infections" scored poorly in memorability.

The researchers further sought to determine factors predicting the effectiveness of AMR-related health terms. "Antibiotic resistance" was selected due to its relevance in the existing literature and high-risk association ratings in the studies. "Drug-resistant infections" was also selected, as it performed best in terms of word effectiveness metrics.

Preliminary tests were conducted to determine correlations between word effectiveness metrics, including risk association and memorability, various linguistic elements, and participant factors, including demographics and medical history. For the term "drug-resistant infections," multiple regression analyses showed that demographic variables, memory scores, and linguistic dimensions significantly impacted risk association scores in both studies. In contrast, for "antibiotic resistance," only linguistic dimensions played a significant role in influencing risk association scores.

When examining memorability, demographic factors and linguistic dimensions were the primary predictors. Notably, men were almost three times more likely than women to recall the term "drug-resistant infections" in the first study. For "antibiotic resistance," having a broader vocabulary and familiarity with the term increased the likelihood of participants remembering it.

Journal reference:
  • Krockow, E. M., Cheng, K. O., Maltby, J., & McElroy, E. (2023). Existing terminology related to antimicrobial resistance fails to evoke risk perceptions and be remembered. Communications Medicine. doi:10.1038/s43856-023-00379-6 
Vijay Kumar Malesu

Written by

Vijay Kumar Malesu

Vijay holds a Ph.D. in Biotechnology and possesses a deep passion for microbiology. His academic journey has allowed him to delve deeper into understanding the intricate world of microorganisms. Through his research and studies, he has gained expertise in various aspects of microbiology, which includes microbial genetics, microbial physiology, and microbial ecology. Vijay has six years of scientific research experience at renowned research institutes such as the Indian Council for Agricultural Research and KIIT University. He has worked on diverse projects in microbiology, biopolymers, and drug delivery. His contributions to these areas have provided him with a comprehensive understanding of the subject matter and the ability to tackle complex research challenges.    

Citations

Please use one of the following formats to cite this article in your essay, paper or report:

  • APA

    Kumar Malesu, Vijay. (2023, October 29). Current jargon on antibiotic resistance fails to stick or scare, study finds. News-Medical. Retrieved on November 22, 2024 from https://www.news-medical.net/news/20231029/Current-jargon-on-antibiotic-resistance-fails-to-stick-or-scare-study-finds.aspx.

  • MLA

    Kumar Malesu, Vijay. "Current jargon on antibiotic resistance fails to stick or scare, study finds". News-Medical. 22 November 2024. <https://www.news-medical.net/news/20231029/Current-jargon-on-antibiotic-resistance-fails-to-stick-or-scare-study-finds.aspx>.

  • Chicago

    Kumar Malesu, Vijay. "Current jargon on antibiotic resistance fails to stick or scare, study finds". News-Medical. https://www.news-medical.net/news/20231029/Current-jargon-on-antibiotic-resistance-fails-to-stick-or-scare-study-finds.aspx. (accessed November 22, 2024).

  • Harvard

    Kumar Malesu, Vijay. 2023. Current jargon on antibiotic resistance fails to stick or scare, study finds. News-Medical, viewed 22 November 2024, https://www.news-medical.net/news/20231029/Current-jargon-on-antibiotic-resistance-fails-to-stick-or-scare-study-finds.aspx.

Comments

The opinions expressed here are the views of the writer and do not necessarily reflect the views and opinions of News Medical.
Post a new comment
Post

While we only use edited and approved content for Azthena answers, it may on occasions provide incorrect responses. Please confirm any data provided with the related suppliers or authors. We do not provide medical advice, if you search for medical information you must always consult a medical professional before acting on any information provided.

Your questions, but not your email details will be shared with OpenAI and retained for 30 days in accordance with their privacy principles.

Please do not ask questions that use sensitive or confidential information.

Read the full Terms & Conditions.

You might also like...
The AI-Powered Lab Assistant with Scientific Awareness